Early
Life
B. F. Skinner was
born on March 20, 1904 in Susquehanna, a small railroad town in the hills of Pennsylvania just below Binghamton, New York.
With one younger brother, he grew up in a home environment he described as "warm and stable". His father was a rising young
lawyer, his mother a housewife. Much of his boyhood was spent building things - for example a cart with steering that worked
backwards (by mistake) and a perpetual motion machine (the latter did not work). Other ventures were more successful. He and
a friend built a cabin in the woods. For a door to door business selling elderberries, he designed a flotation system to separate
ripe from green berries. When working in a shoe store during his high school years, he made a contraption to distribute the
"green dust" that helped the broom pick up dirt.
In high school, Skinner
took an English class taught by Miss Graves to whom he was later to dedicate his book, The Technology of Teaching. Based on a remark by his father, he blurted out in class one day that Shakespeare had not written As You Like It,
but rather Frances Bacon. When his teacher told him he didn't know what he was talking about, he went to the library and read
quite a bit of Bacon's works. Bacon's championing of the inductive method in science against the appeal to authority was to
serve him well later.
First Encounters
with Behavioral Science
After attending Hamilton
college, Skinner decided to become a writer. Moving back home he wrote little. His entire production from the period he called
his "dark year" consisted of a dozen short newspaper articles and a few models of sailing ships. Escaping to New York City
for a few months working as a bookstore clerk, he happened upon books by Pavlov and Watson. He found them impressive and exciting
and wanted to learn more.
Graduate School
and Discovery
At the age of 24
Skinner enrolled in the Psychology Department of Harvard University. Still rebellious and impatient with what he considered
unintelligent ideas, Skinner found a mentor equally caustic and hard-driving. William Crozier was the chair of a new department
of Physiology. Crozier fervently adhered to a program of studying the behavior of "the animal as a whole" without appealing,
as the psychologists did, to processes going on inside. That exactly matched Skinner's goal of relating behavior to experimental
conditions. The student was encouraged to experiment. Each department, Psychology, and Physiology, assumed the other was supervising
the young student, but the fact was he was "doing exactly as I pleased". With his enthusiasm and talent for building new equipment,
Skinner constructed apparatus after apparatus as his rats' behavior suggested changes. After a dozen pieces of apparatus and
some lucky accidents (described in his "A Case History in Scientific Method"), Skinner invented the cumulative recorder, a mechanical device that recorded every response as an upward movement
of a horizontally moving line. The slope showed rate of responding. This recorder revealed the impact of the contingencies
over responding. Skinner discovered that the rate with which the rat pressed the bar depended not on any preceding stimulus
(as Watson and Pavlov had insisted), but on what followed the bar presses. This was new indeed. Unlike the reflexes that Pavlov
had studied, this kind of behavior operated on the environment and was controlled by its effects. Skinner named it operant behavior. The process of arranging the contingencies of reinforcement responsible for producing this new kind of behavior
he called operant conditioning. Because of a fellowship, Skinner was able to spend his next five years investigating not only
the effect of following consequences and the schedules on which they were delivered, but also how prior stimuli gained control
over behavior-consequence relationships with which they were paired. These studies eventually appeared in his first book,
The Behavior of Organisms (1938).
Project Pigeon
In 1936, then 32
years old, Skinner married Yvonne Blue and the couple moved to Minnesota where Skinner had his first teaching job. Busy with
teaching and his new family that in 1938 included a daughter, Julie, he did little to advance the science he had started.
But that was to change with the war. In 1944 World War II was in full swing. Airplanes and bombs were common, but there were
no missile guidance systems. Anxious to help, Skinner sought funding for a top secret project to train pigeons to guide bombs.
Working intently, he trained pigeons to keep pecking a target that would hold a missile onto a target. The pigeons pecked
reliably, even when falling rapidly and working with warlike noise all around them. While Project Pigeon was discontinued
(because of another top secret project unknown to Skinner - radar), the work was useful. Pigeons behave more rapidly than
rats, allowing more rapid discoveries of the effect of new contingencies. As Skinner put it, "the research that I described
in The Behavior of Organisms appeared in a new light. It was no longer merely an experimental analysis. It had given rise to a technology." Skinner
never again worked with rats. Skinner described Project Pigeon in an article with the same name. The article is in
Cumulative Record.
The Baby Tender
In 1943, towards
the end of the Minnesota years, Yvonne was pregnant again. Knowing of her husband's talent at solving problems with gadgets,
she wondered whether he might design a crib that would be safer than the typical crib with its bars that could trap a leg
and blankets that could suffocate a baby. He could, and did. Proud of his new invention, an enclosed and heated crib with
a plexiglass window, he sent an article to the popular magazine the Lady's Home Journal. Changing Skinner's title to
grab attention, the article came out as "Baby in a Box". The "baby tender", as Skinner called his crib, was used only
as a bed for the new baby. Deborah had a playpen and spent as much time out of her bed as do other infants. But inevitably
confusion occurred between the baby tender and the "Skinner Box." To the end of his life Skinner was plagued by rumors about
his second daughter, hearing even that she had committed suicide. In fact, Skinner was an affectionate father and never experimented
on either of his children. Deborah is a successful artist and lives in London with her husband.
Walden Two
When the war was
about to end, Skinner attended a dinner party and mentioned to a friend that it was too bad that her son and other young people
would come back to the old ways of doing things. She asked what Skinner would have them do instead. Skinner could never refuse
a challenge. Almost immediately he began the book Walden Two. Walden Two was written fast, parts with much emotion. In it, a soldier just back from the war, invites friends and his former professor
to visit a community called Walden Two, a group of about 1000 members. They go to the community. Its designer, Frazier, explains
how the happy and the industrious behaviors they are seeing have been carefully shaped using behavioral techniques. He explains
how the competitive urge of parents to favor their own children has been converted to a more equal concern for all youngsters
by bringing up the babies communally rather than in families. Both women and men work. Jobs earn work credits weighted so
that one can work for only a short time at undesirable jobs or longer at desirable ones. All aspects of the community have
been planned. The book, after a slow start, became one of the best known works of Skinner's, receiving both praise and condemnation.
Indiana
In 1945, Skinner
and his family moved to Bloomington Indiana where he became Chair of the Psychology Department at the University of Indiana.
The field he had started was growing. In 1946 the first meeting of the Society of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior was
held in Indiana. Twelve years later, it had a journal, the Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior.
Return to Harvard
An invitation from
Harvard University to give the William James Lectures brought Skinner and his family to Cambridge Massachusetts in the fall
of 1947. He was invited to join the Psychology Department in 1948. There, he offered to give a course for undergraduates,
scrambling each week to produce materials for the 400 students who enrolled. The material eventually became the book Science and Human Behavior (1953). The 1950's and 1960's were productive for Skinner, largely due to the excellent graduate students who came to
study with him, including Douglas B. Anger, James A Anliker, Nathan H. Azrin, Donald S. Blough, A. Charles Catania, Lewis
R. Gollub, Richard J. Herrnstein, Matthew L. Israel, Alfredo V. Lagmay, Harlan I. Lane, Ogden R. Lindlsey, William H. Morse,
Neil J. Peterson, George S. Reynolds, and Herbert S. Terrace. Spinoffs from this work produced the book Schedules of Reinforcement (Ferster & Skinner, 1957), the field of Behavior Therapy (from the work of Ogden Lindsley who coined the term), and
psychopharmacology (where Peter Dews at the Harvard Medical School worked closely with Skinner's lab).
Teaching Machines
and Programmed Instruction
Skinner's children
were growing up. When the younger was in fourth grade, on November 11, 1953, Skinner attended her math class for Father's
Day. The visit altered his life. As he sat at the back of that typical fourth grade math class, what he saw suddenly hit him
with the force of an inspiration. As he put it, "through no fault of her own the teacher was violating almost everything we
knew about the learning process." In shaping, you adapt what you ask of an animal to the animal's current performance level.
But in the math class, clearly some of the students had no idea of how to solve the problems, while others whipped through
the exercise sheet, learning nothing new. In shaping, each best response is immediately reinforced. Skinner had researched
delay of reinforcement and knew how it hampered performance. But in the math class, the children did not find out if one problem
was correct before doing the next. They had to answer a whole page before getting any feedback, and then probably not until
the next day. But how could one teacher with 20 or 30 children possibly shape mathematical behavior in each one? Clearly teachers
needed help. That afternoon, Skinner constructed his first teaching machine.
Skinner's first teaching
machine simply presented problems in random order for students to do, with feedback after each one. But this machine did not
teach new behavior. All it did was give more practice on skills already learned. Within three years, however, Skinner developed
programmed instruction, where through careful sequencing, students responded to material broken into small steps. The
steps were similar to what a skilled tutor would ask of a student working with one student at a time. The first responses
of each sequence were prompted, but as performance improved, less and less help was given. By the end, a student was doing
something he or she could not have done at the beginning. For about ten years, Skinner was caught up in the teaching machine
movement, answering every one of thousands of letters from parents, schools, and business and industry. With a grant, Skinner
hired James G. Holland who with Skinner's supervision, created The Analysis of Behavior for Skinner's class of Harvard students to take on a mechanical machine. (There were no microcomputers yet.) The field
of education embraced this newest teaching method, but many of the materials were poorly written and no company wanted to
design materials for a teaching machine that might go out of production. So most programmed instruction was put into book
form. But the a book does not maintain the contingencies: Students can look at the answer before writing their own. By around
1968 education publishers stopped printing programmed instruction. That same year Skinner published The Technology of Teaching, a collection of his writings on education. Some of the better programs from the 60's are still used and with the coming
of the computer and internet, the perfect machine that Skinner lacked is now available. Increasingly, instructional designers
are realizing that, as Skinner insisted, tutorials must do more than present blocks of content with quizzes at the end. Effective
instruction requires learners to respond to what each screen of information presents and to get feedback on their performance
before advancing to the next. In addition, the sequencing of steps is critical.
Skinner's analysis
of how to design sequences of steps came to him as he was finishing a book on which he had worked, on and off, for twenty
years. Verbal Behavior published in 1957 is an analysis of why we say, write, and even think the way we do. The book is not easy to read. It
took another twenty years before researchers used Skinner's categories and found that the different controlling variables
he postulated were, indeed, independent. Recently their work has led to breakthroughs in teaching children, especially those
with autism, to communicate effectively.
Later Life
A concern with the
implications of behavioral science for society at large turned Skinner to philosophical and moral issues. In 1969 he published
Contingencies of Reinforcement and two years later Beyond Freedom and Dignity which prompted a series of television appearances. Still, the lack of understanding and misrepresentation of his work
prompted his writing About Behaviorism (1974). Towards the end of his life he was still active professionally. In addition to professional articles, he wrote
three autobiographical volumes, Particulars of my Life, The Shaping of a Behaviorist, and A Matter of Consequences. In 1989 he was diagnosed with leukemia, but kept as active as his increasing weakness allowed. At the American Psychological
Association, ten days before he died, he gave a talk before a crowded auditorium. He finished the article from which the talk
was taken on August 18, 1990, the day he died.
Contributed by B. F. Skinner's elder daughter